Actions speak louder than gestures when you are 2 years old

Miriam A. Novack*, Courtney A. Filippi, Susan Goldin-Meadow, Amanda L. Woodward

*Corresponding author for this work

Research output: Contribution to journalArticlepeer-review

9 Scopus citations

Abstract

Interpreting iconic gestures can be challenging for children. Here, we explore the features and functions of iconic gestures that make them more challenging for young children to interpret than instrumental actions. In Study 1, we show that 2.5-year-olds are able to glean size information from handshape in a simple gesture, although their performance is significantly worse than 4-year-olds'. Studies 2 to 4 explore the boundary conditions of 2.5-year-olds' gesture understanding. In Study 2, 2.5-year-old children have an easier time interpreting size information in hands that reach than in hands that gesture. In Study 3, we tease apart the perceptual features and functional objectives of reaches and gestures. We created a context in which an action has the perceptual features of a reach (extending the hand toward an object) but serves the function of a gesture (the object is behind a barrier and not obtainable; the hand thus functions to represent, rather than reach for, the object). In this context, children struggle to interpret size information in the hand, suggesting that gesture's representational function (rather than its perceptual features) is what makes it hard for young children to interpret. A distance control (Study 4) in which a person holds a box in gesture space (close to the body) demonstrates that children's difficulty interpreting static gesture cannot be attributed to the physical distance between a gesture and its referent. Together, these studies provide evidence that children's struggle to interpret iconic gesture may stem from its status as representational action.

Original languageEnglish (US)
Pages (from-to)1809-1821
Number of pages13
JournalDevelopmental psychology
Volume54
Issue number10
DOIs
StatePublished - Oct 2018

Funding

This work was supported by National Institutes of Health (NIH) grant National Institute of Health and Human Development (NICHD P01 HD064653) to Amanda L. Woodward, and NICHD R01-HD47450 and National Science Foundation (NSF) Grant BCS-0925595 to Susan Goldin-Meadow and (in part) by the Intramural Research Program of the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) in support of Courtney A. Filippi. This article was prepared while Courtney A. Filippi was employed at the University of Chicago. The opinions expressed in this article are the authors’ own and do not reflect the views of NIH or the Federal Government. This work was supported by National Institutes of Health (NIH) grant National Institute of Health and Human Development (NICHD P01 HD064653) to Amanda L. Woodward, and NICHD R01-HD47450 and National Science Foundation (NSF) Grant BCS-0925595 to Susan Goldin-Meadow and (in part) by the Intramural Research Program of the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) in support of Courtney A. Filippi. This article was prepared while Courtney A. Filippi was employed at the University of Chicago. The opinions expressed in this article are the authors' own and do not reflect the views of NIH or the Federal Government.

Keywords

  • Action
  • Gesture
  • Iconicity
  • Representation

ASJC Scopus subject areas

  • Demography
  • Developmental and Educational Psychology
  • Life-span and Life-course Studies

Fingerprint

Dive into the research topics of 'Actions speak louder than gestures when you are 2 years old'. Together they form a unique fingerprint.

Cite this