TY - JOUR
T1 - Children's Understanding of the Natural Numbers’ Structure
AU - Asmuth, Jennifer
AU - Morson, Emily M.
AU - Rips, Lance J.
N1 - Funding Information:
We thank Nathan Couch, Jacob Dink, Véronique Izard, David Landy, Nicholas Leonard, Matthew Myers, and Caren Rotello for valuable suggestions; Elaine Anderson, Kevin Bleistein, Jennifer Bohnert, Ashley Machamer, and Kirsten Youse for their assistance with recruiting and running subjects, as well as coding data; and the participating children and parents. We also thank audiences at the State University of New York at Stony Brook, at Northwestern University, at a Cognitive Science Society workshop, and at the conference of the Society for Research on Child Development for their comments. Partial funding of this research was provided by a Susquehanna University Committee on Faculty Scholarship grant to JA.
Funding Information:
We thank Nathan Couch, Jacob Dink, V?ronique Izard, David Landy, Nicholas Leonard, Matthew Myers, and Caren Rotello for valuable suggestions; Elaine Anderson, Kevin Bleistein, Jennifer Bohnert, Ashley Machamer, and Kirsten Youse for their assistance with recruiting and running subjects, as well as coding data; and the participating children and parents. We also thank audiences at the State University of New York at Stony Brook, at Northwestern University, at a Cognitive Science Society workshop, and at the conference of the Society for Research on Child Development for their comments. Partial funding of this research was provided by a Susquehanna University Committee on Faculty Scholarship grant to JA.
Publisher Copyright:
Copyright © 2018 Cognitive Science Society, Inc.
PY - 2018/8
Y1 - 2018/8
N2 - When young children attempt to locate numbers along a number line, they show logarithmic (or other compressive) placement. For example, the distance between “5” and “10” is larger than the distance between “75” and “80.” This has often been explained by assuming that children have a logarithmically scaled mental representation of number (e.g., Berteletti, Lucangeli, Piazza, Dehaene, & Zorzi,; Siegler & Opfer,). However, several investigators have questioned this argument (e.g., Barth & Paladino,; Cantlon, Cordes, Libertus, & Brannon,; Cohen & Blanc-Goldhammer,). We show here that children prefer linear number lines over logarithmic lines when they do not have to deal with the meanings of individual numerals (i.e., number symbols, such as “5” or “80”). In Experiments 1 and 2, when 5- and 6-year-olds choose between number lines in a forced-choice task, they prefer linear to logarithmic and exponential displays. However, this preference does not persist when Experiment 3 presents the same lines without reference to numbers, and children simply choose which line they like best. In Experiments 4 and 5, children position beads on a number line to indicate how the integers 1–100 are arranged. The bead placement of 4- and 5-year-olds is better fit by a linear than by a logarithmic model. We argue that previous results from the number-line task may depend on strategies specific to the task.
AB - When young children attempt to locate numbers along a number line, they show logarithmic (or other compressive) placement. For example, the distance between “5” and “10” is larger than the distance between “75” and “80.” This has often been explained by assuming that children have a logarithmically scaled mental representation of number (e.g., Berteletti, Lucangeli, Piazza, Dehaene, & Zorzi,; Siegler & Opfer,). However, several investigators have questioned this argument (e.g., Barth & Paladino,; Cantlon, Cordes, Libertus, & Brannon,; Cohen & Blanc-Goldhammer,). We show here that children prefer linear number lines over logarithmic lines when they do not have to deal with the meanings of individual numerals (i.e., number symbols, such as “5” or “80”). In Experiments 1 and 2, when 5- and 6-year-olds choose between number lines in a forced-choice task, they prefer linear to logarithmic and exponential displays. However, this preference does not persist when Experiment 3 presents the same lines without reference to numbers, and children simply choose which line they like best. In Experiments 4 and 5, children position beads on a number line to indicate how the integers 1–100 are arranged. The bead placement of 4- and 5-year-olds is better fit by a linear than by a logarithmic model. We argue that previous results from the number-line task may depend on strategies specific to the task.
KW - Natural numbers
KW - Number concepts
KW - Number development
KW - Number line
KW - Numerical cognition
UR - http://www.scopus.com/inward/record.url?scp=85050385723&partnerID=8YFLogxK
UR - http://www.scopus.com/inward/citedby.url?scp=85050385723&partnerID=8YFLogxK
U2 - 10.1111/cogs.12615
DO - 10.1111/cogs.12615
M3 - Article
C2 - 29974507
AN - SCOPUS:85050385723
SN - 0364-0213
VL - 42
SP - 1945
EP - 1973
JO - Cognitive Science
JF - Cognitive Science
IS - 6
ER -