Abstract
Given the powerful implications of relationship quality for health and well-being, a central mission of relationship science is explaining why some romantic relationships thrive more than others. This large-scale project used machine learning (i.e., Random Forests) to 1) quantify the extent to which relationship quality is predictable and 2) identify which constructs reliably predict relationship quality. Across 43 dyadic longitudinal datasets from 29 laboratories, the top relationship-specific predictors of relationship quality were perceived-partner commitment, appreciation, sexual satisfaction, perceived-partner satisfaction, and conflict. The top individual-difference predictors were life satisfaction, negative affect, depression, attachment avoidance, and attachment anxiety. Overall, relationship-specific variables predicted up to 45% of variance at baseline, and up to 18% of variance at the end of each study. Individual differences also performed well (21% and 12%, respectively). Actor-reported variables (i.e., own relationship-specific and individual-difference variables) predicted two to four times more variance than partner-reported variables (i.e., the partner's ratings on those variables). Importantly, individual differences and partner reports had no predictive effects beyond actor-reported relationship-specific variables alone. These findings imply that the sum of all individual differences and partner experiences exert their influence on relationship quality via a person's own relationship-specific experiences, and effects due to moderation by individual differences and moderation by partner-reports may be quite small. Finally, relationship-quality change (i.e., increases or decreases in relationship quality over the course of a study) was largely unpredictable from any combination of self-report variables. This collective effort should guide future models of relationships.
Original language | English (US) |
---|---|
Pages (from-to) | 19061-19071 |
Number of pages | 11 |
Journal | Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America |
Volume | 117 |
Issue number | 32 |
DOIs | |
State | Published - Aug 11 2020 |
Keywords
- Ensemble methods
- Machine learning
- Random Forests
- Relationship quality
- Romantic relationships
ASJC Scopus subject areas
- General
Access to Document
Other files and links
Fingerprint
Dive into the research topics of 'Machine learning uncovers the most robust self-report predictors of relationship quality across 43 longitudinal couples studies'. Together they form a unique fingerprint.Cite this
- APA
- Author
- BIBTEX
- Harvard
- Standard
- RIS
- Vancouver
}
In: Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, Vol. 117, No. 32, 11.08.2020, p. 19061-19071.
Research output: Contribution to journal › Article › peer-review
TY - JOUR
T1 - Machine learning uncovers the most robust self-report predictors of relationship quality across 43 longitudinal couples studies
AU - Joel, Samantha
AU - Eastwick, Paul W.
AU - Allison, Colleen J.
AU - Arriaga, Ximena B.
AU - Baker, Zachary G.
AU - Bar-Kalifa, Eran
AU - Bergeron, Sophie
AU - Birnbaum, Gurit E.
AU - Brock, Rebecca L.
AU - Brumbaugh, Claudia C.
AU - Carmichael, Cheryl L.
AU - Chen, Serena
AU - Clarke, Jennifer
AU - Cobb, Rebecca J.
AU - Coolsen, Michael K.
AU - Davis, Jody
AU - de Jong, David C.
AU - Debrot, Anik
AU - DeHaas, Eva C.
AU - Derrick, Jaye L.
AU - Eller, Jami
AU - Estrada, Marie Joelle
AU - Faure, Ruddy
AU - Finkel, Eli J.
AU - Chris Fraley, R.
AU - Gable, Shelly L.
AU - Gadassi-Polack, Reuma
AU - Girme, Yuthika U.
AU - Gordon, Amie M.
AU - Gosnell, Courtney L.
AU - Hammond, Matthew D.
AU - Hannon, Peggy A.
AU - Harasymchuk, Cheryl
AU - Hofmann, Wilhelm
AU - Horn, Andrea B.
AU - Impett, Emily A.
AU - Jamieson, Jeremy P.
AU - Keltner, Dacher
AU - Kim, James J.
AU - Kirchner, Jeffrey L.
AU - Kluwer, Esther S.
AU - Kumashiro, Madoka
AU - Larson, Grace
AU - Lazarus, Gal
AU - Logan, Jill M.
AU - Luchies, Laura B.
AU - MacDonald, Geoff
AU - Machia, Laura V.
AU - Maniaci, Michael R.
AU - Maxwell, Jessica A.
AU - Mizrahi, Moran
AU - Muise, Amy
AU - Niehuis, Sylvia
AU - Ogolsky, Brian G.
AU - Rebecca Oldham, C.
AU - Overall, Nickola C.
AU - Perrez, Meinrad
AU - Peters, Brett J.
AU - Pietromonaco, Paula R.
AU - Powers, Sally I.
AU - Prok, Thery
AU - Pshedetzky-Shochat, Rony
AU - Rafaeli, Eshkol
AU - Ramsdell, Erin L.
AU - Reblin, Maija
AU - Reicherts, Michael
AU - Reifman, Alan
AU - Reis, Harry T.
AU - Rhoades, Galena K.
AU - Rholes, William S.
AU - Righetti, Francesca
AU - Rodriguez, Lindsey M.
AU - Rogge, Ron
AU - Rosen, Natalie O.
AU - Saxbe, Darby
AU - Sened, Haran
AU - Simpson, Jeffry A.
AU - Slotter, Erica B.
AU - Stanley, Scott M.
AU - Stocker, Shevaun
AU - Surra, Cathy
AU - Kuile, Hagar Ter
AU - Vaughn, Allison A.
AU - Vicary, Amanda M.
AU - Visserman, Mariko L.
AU - Wolf, Scott
N1 - Funding Information: ACKNOWLEDGMENTS. The work of aggregating the datasets was supported by a Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada Insight Grant 435-2019-0115 (to S.J.). Collection of the 43 datasets was supported by many separate funding sources. Datasets 1 and 2 were funded by NSF Grant BCS-719780 (to E.J.F.). Dataset 3 was partially supported by a National Research Service Predoctoral Training Grant (to S.L.G.). Dataset 4 was supported by a Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council (SSHRC) Predoctoral fellowship (to D.C.d.J.). Dataset 5 was funded by a grant from the Fetzer Institute (to H.T.R.). Datasets 8 and 9 were supported by an SSHRC Banting postdoctoral fellowship, and Dataset 10 was supported by an SSHRC doctoral fellowship (to A.M.). Dataset 13 was funded by University of Auckland Doctoral Research funds (Y.U.G. and M.D.H.). Dataset 14 was funded by University of Auckland Grants 3626244 and 3607021 (to N.C.O.). Dataset 15 was funded by the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism under Award F31AA020442 (to L.M.R.). Dataset 16 was funded by National Cancer Institute Grant R01CA133908 (to P.R.P. and S.I.P.). Dataset 17 was funded by NSF Grant BCS-0443783 (to R.C.F.). Dataset 18 was funded by National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) Grant BSR–R01-MH-45417 (to Caryl E. Rusbult). Dataset 19 was funded by the Clayton Award for Excellence in Graduate Research from the University of Utah (to A.A.V. and M. R. Reblin). Dataset 20 was funded by NIMH Grant MH49599 (to J.A.S. and W.S.R.). Dataset 22 was funded by a joint Open Research Area grant from the Dutch Science Foundation 464-15-093 (to F.R.) and HO4175/6-1 from the German Science Foundation (to W.H.). Dataset 24 was funded by a Utrecht University High Potential grant (to E.S.K.). Dataset 25 was funded by Israel Science Foundation Grant 615/10 (to E..R.) and work on its adaptation to this study was supported by Azrieli Foundation fellowships (to G. Lazarus and H.S.). Datasets 27 and 28 were funded by Texas Tech University’s Office of the Vice President for Research, Office of Institutional Diversity, Equity, and Community Engagement, and College of Human Sciences (S.N.). Dataset 29 was funded by the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development under Award HD047564 (to S.M.S.). Dataset 30 was funded by SSHRC Insight Development Grant 430-2016-00422 (to C.H., A.M., and E.A.I.). Dataset 31 was funded by the Swiss National Science Foundation and was part of National Center of Competence in Research of Affective Sciences Grant 51A24-104897 (to M.P. and M. Reicherts). Dataset 32 was funded by the National Research University Fund, Division of Research, University of Houston, and a University of Houston CLASS Research Progress Grant (to J.D.) and National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism of the National Institutes of Health under Award F31AA026195 (to Z.G.B.). Dataset 33 was funded by a Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR) postdoctoral fellowship (to N.O.R.) and a CIHR grant (to S.B.). Dataset 34 was funded by a CIHR grant (to N.O.R.). Dataset 37 was funded by NSF Grant BCS-1050875 (to S.L.G.). Dataset 39 was funded by NSF Grant BCS-0132398 (to Caryl E. Rusbult). Dataset 40 was funded by Templeton Foundation Grant 5158 (to Caryl E. Rusbult). Dataset 41 was funded by an SSHRC Canadian Graduate Scholarship (to J.A.M.) and an SSHRC Insight grant (to G.M.). Dataset 42 was funded by SSHRC Research Grant 410-2005-0829 (to R.J.C.). Funding Information: The work of aggregating the datasets was supported by a Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada Insight Grant 435-2019-0115 (to S.J.). Collection of the 43 datasets was supported by many separate funding sources. Datasets 1 and 2 were funded by NSF Grant BCS-719780 (to E.J.F.). Dataset 3 was partially supported by a National Research Service Predoctoral Training Grant (to S.L.G.). Dataset 4 was supported by a Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council (SSHRC) Predoctoral fellowship (to D.C.d.J.). Dataset 5 was funded by a grant from the Fetzer Institute (to H.T.R.). Datasets 8 and 9 were supported by an SSHRC Banting postdoctoral fellowship, and Dataset 10 was supported by an SSHRC doctoral fellowship (to A.M.). Dataset 13 was funded by University of Auckland Doctoral Research funds (Y.U.G. and M.D.H.). Dataset 14 was funded by University of Auckland Grants 3626244 and 3607021 (to N.C.O.). Dataset 15 was funded by the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism under Award F31AA020442 (to L.M.R.). Dataset 16 was funded by National Cancer Institute Grant R01CA133908 (to P.R.P. and S.I.P.). Dataset 17 was funded by NSF Grant BCS-0443783 (to R.C.F.). Dataset 18 was funded by National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) Grant BSR-R01-MH-45417 (to Caryl E. Rusbult). Dataset 19 was funded by the Clayton Award for Excellence in Graduate Research from the University of Utah (to A.A.V. and M. R. Reblin). Dataset 20 was funded by NIMH Grant MH49599 (to J.A.S. and W.S.R.). Dataset 22 was funded by a joint Open Research Area grant from the Dutch Science Foundation 464-15-093 (to F.R.) and HO4175/6-1 from the German Science Foundation (to W.H.). Dataset 24 was funded by a Utrecht University High Potential grant (to E.S.K.). Dataset 25 was funded by Israel Science Foundation Grant 615/10 (to E..R.) and work on its adaptation to this study was supported by Azrieli Foundation fellowships (to G. Lazarus and H.S.). Datasets 27 and 28 were funded by Texas Tech University's Office of the Vice President for Research, Office of Institutional Diversity, Equity, and Community Engagement, and College of Human Sciences (S.N.). Dataset 29 was funded by the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development under Award HD047564 (to S.M.S.). Dataset 30 was funded by SSHRC Insight Development Grant 430-2016-00422 (to C.H., A.M., and E.A.I.). Dataset 31 was funded by the Swiss National Science Foundation and was part of National Center of Competence in Research of Affective Sciences Grant 51A24-104897 (to M.P. and M. Reicherts). Dataset 32 was funded by the National Research University Fund, Division of Research, University of Houston, and a University of Houston CLASS Research Progress Grant (to J.D.) and National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism of the National Institutes of Health under Award F31AA026195 (to Z.G.B.). Dataset 33 was funded by a Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR) postdoctoral fellowship (to N.O.R.) and a CIHR grant (to S.B.). Dataset 34 was funded by a CIHR grant (to N.O.R.). Dataset 37 was funded by NSF Grant BCS-1050875 (to S.L.G.). Dataset 39 was funded by NSF Grant BCS-0132398 (to Caryl E. Rusbult). Dataset 40 was funded by Templeton Foundation Grant 5158 (to Caryl E. Rusbult). Dataset 41 was funded by an SSHRC Canadian Graduate Scholarship (to J.A.M.) and an SSHRC Insight grant (to G.M.). Dataset 42 was funded by SSHRC Research Grant 410-2005-0829 (to R.J.C.). Publisher Copyright: © 2020 National Academy of Sciences. All rights reserved.
PY - 2020/8/11
Y1 - 2020/8/11
N2 - Given the powerful implications of relationship quality for health and well-being, a central mission of relationship science is explaining why some romantic relationships thrive more than others. This large-scale project used machine learning (i.e., Random Forests) to 1) quantify the extent to which relationship quality is predictable and 2) identify which constructs reliably predict relationship quality. Across 43 dyadic longitudinal datasets from 29 laboratories, the top relationship-specific predictors of relationship quality were perceived-partner commitment, appreciation, sexual satisfaction, perceived-partner satisfaction, and conflict. The top individual-difference predictors were life satisfaction, negative affect, depression, attachment avoidance, and attachment anxiety. Overall, relationship-specific variables predicted up to 45% of variance at baseline, and up to 18% of variance at the end of each study. Individual differences also performed well (21% and 12%, respectively). Actor-reported variables (i.e., own relationship-specific and individual-difference variables) predicted two to four times more variance than partner-reported variables (i.e., the partner's ratings on those variables). Importantly, individual differences and partner reports had no predictive effects beyond actor-reported relationship-specific variables alone. These findings imply that the sum of all individual differences and partner experiences exert their influence on relationship quality via a person's own relationship-specific experiences, and effects due to moderation by individual differences and moderation by partner-reports may be quite small. Finally, relationship-quality change (i.e., increases or decreases in relationship quality over the course of a study) was largely unpredictable from any combination of self-report variables. This collective effort should guide future models of relationships.
AB - Given the powerful implications of relationship quality for health and well-being, a central mission of relationship science is explaining why some romantic relationships thrive more than others. This large-scale project used machine learning (i.e., Random Forests) to 1) quantify the extent to which relationship quality is predictable and 2) identify which constructs reliably predict relationship quality. Across 43 dyadic longitudinal datasets from 29 laboratories, the top relationship-specific predictors of relationship quality were perceived-partner commitment, appreciation, sexual satisfaction, perceived-partner satisfaction, and conflict. The top individual-difference predictors were life satisfaction, negative affect, depression, attachment avoidance, and attachment anxiety. Overall, relationship-specific variables predicted up to 45% of variance at baseline, and up to 18% of variance at the end of each study. Individual differences also performed well (21% and 12%, respectively). Actor-reported variables (i.e., own relationship-specific and individual-difference variables) predicted two to four times more variance than partner-reported variables (i.e., the partner's ratings on those variables). Importantly, individual differences and partner reports had no predictive effects beyond actor-reported relationship-specific variables alone. These findings imply that the sum of all individual differences and partner experiences exert their influence on relationship quality via a person's own relationship-specific experiences, and effects due to moderation by individual differences and moderation by partner-reports may be quite small. Finally, relationship-quality change (i.e., increases or decreases in relationship quality over the course of a study) was largely unpredictable from any combination of self-report variables. This collective effort should guide future models of relationships.
KW - Ensemble methods
KW - Machine learning
KW - Random Forests
KW - Relationship quality
KW - Romantic relationships
UR - http://www.scopus.com/inward/record.url?scp=85089613576&partnerID=8YFLogxK
UR - http://www.scopus.com/inward/citedby.url?scp=85089613576&partnerID=8YFLogxK
U2 - 10.1073/pnas.1917036117
DO - 10.1073/pnas.1917036117
M3 - Article
C2 - 32719123
AN - SCOPUS:85089613576
SN - 0027-8424
VL - 117
SP - 19061
EP - 19071
JO - Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
JF - Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
IS - 32
ER -