Many analysts, one data set: Making transparent how variations in analytic choices affect results

R. Silberzahn*, E. L. Uhlmann*, D. P. Martin*, P. Anselmi, F. Aust, E. Awtrey, Bahník, F. Bai, C. Bannard, E. Bonnier, R. Carlsson, F. Cheung, G. Christensen, R. Clay, M. A. Craig, A. Dalla Rosa, L. Dam, M. H. Evans, I. Flores Cervantes, N. FongM. Gamez-Djokic, A. Glenz, S. Gordon-Mckeon, T. J. Heaton, K. Hederos, M. Heene, A. J. Hofelich Mohr, F. Högden, K. Hui, M. Johannesson, J. Kalodimos, E. Kaszubowski, D. M. Kennedy, R. Lei, T. A. Lindsay, S. Liverani, C. R. Madan, D. Molden, E. Molleman, R. D. Morey, L. B. Mulder, B. R. Nijstad, N. G. Pope, B. Pope, J. M. Prenoveau, F. Rink, E. Robusto, H. Roderique, A. Sandberg, E. Schlüter, F. D. Schönbrodt, M. F. Sherman, S. A. Sommer, K. Sotak, S. Spain, C. Spörlein, T. Stafford, L. Stefanutti, S. Tauber, J. Ullrich, M. Vianello, E. J. Wagenmakers, M. Witkowiak, S. Yoon, B. A. Nosek*

*Corresponding author for this work

Research output: Contribution to journalArticlepeer-review

420 Scopus citations

Abstract

Twenty-nine teams involving 61 analysts used the same data set to address the same research question: whether soccer referees are more likely to give red cards to dark-skin-toned players than to light-skin-toned players. Analytic approaches varied widely across the teams, and the estimated effect sizes ranged from 0.89 to 2.93 (Mdn = 1.31) in odds-ratio units. Twenty teams (69%) found a statistically significant positive effect, and 9 teams (31%) did not observe a significant relationship. Overall, the 29 different analyses used 21 unique combinations of covariates. Neither analysts’ prior beliefs about the effect of interest nor their level of expertise readily explained the variation in the outcomes of the analyses. Peer ratings of the quality of the analyses also did not account for the variability. These findings suggest that significant variation in the results of analyses of complex data may be difficult to avoid, even by experts with honest intentions. Crowdsourcing data analysis, a strategy in which numerous research teams are recruited to simultaneously investigate the same research question, makes transparent how defensible, yet subjective, analytic choices influence research results.

Original languageEnglish (US)
Pages (from-to)337-356
Number of pages20
JournalAdvances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science
Volume1
Issue number3
DOIs
StatePublished - 2018

Funding

D. P. Martin was supported by the Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education (Grant No. R305B090002). The contribution of R. D. Morey and E.-J. Wagenmakers was supported by a grant from the European Research Council (Grant No. 283876). M. Johannesson received funding from the Jan Wallander and Tom Hedelius Foundation (Grant No. P2015-0001:1), as well as from the Swedish Foundation for Humanities and Social Sciences (Grant No. NHS14-1719:1). S. Liverani was supported by a Leverhulme Trust Early Career Fellowship (Grant No. ECF-2011-576). C. R. Madan was supported by a Canadian Graduate Scholarship, Doctoral-level, from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (Grant No. CGSD2-426287-2012). T. Stafford was supported by a Leverhulme Trust Research Project Grant (Grant No. RPG2013-326).

Keywords

  • Crowdsourcing science
  • Data analysis
  • Open data
  • Open materials
  • Scientific transparency

ASJC Scopus subject areas

  • General Psychology

Fingerprint

Dive into the research topics of 'Many analysts, one data set: Making transparent how variations in analytic choices affect results'. Together they form a unique fingerprint.
  • Many Analysts, One Data Set: Making Transparent How Variations in Analytic Choices Affect Results

    Silberzahn, R. (Contributor), Uhlmann, E. L. (Contributor), Martin, D. P. (Contributor), Anselmi, P. (Contributor), Aust, F. (Contributor), Awtrey, E. (Contributor), Bahník, S. (Contributor), Bai, F. (Contributor), Bannard, C. (Contributor), Bonnier, E. (Contributor), Carlsson, R. (Contributor), Cheung, F. (Contributor), Christensen, G. (Contributor), Clay, R. (Contributor), Craig, M. A. (Contributor), Rosa, A. D. (Contributor), Dam, L. (Contributor), Evans, M. H. (Contributor), Cervantes, I. F. (Contributor), Fong, N. (Contributor), Gamez-Djokic, M. (Contributor), Glenz, A. (Contributor), Gordon-Mckeon, S. (Contributor), Heaton, T. J. (Contributor), Hederos, K. (Contributor), Heene, M. (Contributor), Hofelich Mohr, M. A. J. (Contributor), Högden, F. (Contributor), Hui, K. (Contributor), Johannesson, M. (Contributor), Kalodimos, J. (Contributor), Kaszubowski, E. (Contributor), Kennedy, D. M. (Contributor), Lei, R. (Contributor), Lindsay, T. A. (Contributor), Liverani, S. (Contributor), Madan, C. R. (Contributor), Molden, D. (Contributor), Molleman, E. (Contributor), Morey, R. D. (Contributor), Mulder, L. B. (Contributor), Nijstad, B. R. (Contributor), Pope, N. G. (Contributor), Pope, B. (Contributor), Prenoveau, J. M. (Contributor), Rink, F. (Contributor), Robusto, E. (Contributor), Roderique, H. (Contributor), Sandberg, A. (Contributor), Schlüter, E. (Contributor), Schönbrodt, F. D. (Contributor), Sherman, M. F. (Contributor), Sommer, S. A. (Contributor), Sotak, K. (Contributor), Spain, S. (Contributor), Spörlein, C. (Contributor), Stafford, T. (Contributor), Stefanutti, L. (Contributor), Tauber, S. (Contributor), Ullrich, J. (Contributor), Vianello, M. (Contributor), Wagenmakers, E.-J. (Contributor), Witkowiak, M. (Contributor), Yoon, S. (Contributor) & Nosek, B. A. (Contributor), figshare, 2018

    Dataset

Cite this